2.0 CE Credits - Special Issue: The Neuropsychology of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (JINS 24:9, 2018): CE Bundle 3

apa-logo_white_screenThe International Neuropsychological Society is approved by the American Psychological Association to sponsor continuing education for psychologists. The International Neuropsychological Society maintains responsibility for this program and its content.
Educational Objectives
  1. Discuss executive function skills documented in children with neurofibromatosis type 1.
  2. Describe moderator variables of executive function skills in children with in neurofibromatosis type 1.
  3. Describe the course of cortical development within a developmental context in pediatric Down syndrome.
  4. Discuss current limitations and future directions critical for future work in this field (i.e., the field of pediatric Down syndrome).
  5. Discuss the relationship between a molecular lesion approach to dystrophinopathy and cognition.
  6. Explain how individual capacity as opposed to IQ in isolation may be a better predictor of academic achievement.
  7. Describe the memory deficits associated with Down syndrome.
  8. Discuss procedures for learning new words, including fast mapping and explicit encoding.

Course Information
Target Audience:Intermediate
Availability:Date Available: 2019-04-01
You may obtain CE for this JINS package at any time.
Offered for CEYes
CostMembers $20
Non-Members $30
Refund PolicyThis JINS package is not eligible for refunds
CE Credits2.0

Introduction

Neurodevelopmental disorders are conditions that involve early insult or abnormality in the developing central nervous system and are associated with a wide spectrum of abilities. These conditions begin during the early developmental period (usually conceptualized as prenatally though the first 3 years of life), affect day-to-day functioning, and are often lifelong. Because the “typical” development of the nervous system has been altered in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders, reorganization and competition for function occur, usually resulting in skill patterns that are less efficient than among individuals without such conditions. The timing of these alterations or developmental disruptions is also relevant, as different neural systems are selectively vulnerable to injury at different phases of prenatal and post-natal development. As a result, the behavioral and cognitive dysfunction associated with early neural damage can range from subtle (or absent) to diffuse and profound. Moreover, the functional impairments can be observed immediately in some individuals, while in others, the full range of deficits may not manifest until later in life, even though the neurobiological basis of the condition is present earlier (Rudel, 1981).

Among children with neurodevelopmental disorders, the trajectory is often “off developmental track” relative to the trajectory of typically developing children. Developmental delays (i.e., patterns of skill development that should have occurred earlier in life) are often observed early in life. While functional catch-up is possible, it is often incomplete, and the resulting maturational timelines based on typical development become less applicable (Mahone, Slomine, & Zabel, 2018).

Neurodevelopmental disorders are highly prevalent. Recent estimates from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in the United States show that around one in six, or approximately 17%, of children ages 3 through 17 years have one or more neurodevelopmental disabilities (Boyle et al., 2011). The rates also are increasing, and the CDC reports may underestimate the actual prevalence worldwide. In the past 25 years, medical advances have improved the life course of several genetic, medical, and neurodevelopmental conditions, making them more survivable and compatible with life (e.g., very low birth weight preterm infants, congenital hydrocephalus) and extending the expected lifespan of others (e.g., cystic fibrosis, sickle cell disease). Due to higher survival rates and lifespans extending into adulthood, increased attention has been given to the development of self-management and independence skills and the transition into older adolescence and young adulthood (Tarazi, Mahone, & Zabel, 2007; Warschausky, Kaufman, Evitts, Schutt, & Hurvitz, 2017; Zabel, Jacobson, & Mahone, 2013). Given these considerations, the assessment and study of individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders is of significant interest to neuropsychologists.

Classification of neurodevelopmental disorders can be conceptualized using two primary approaches, one emphasizing behavior (without explicit reference to etiology), and the other emphasizing etiological medical, genetic, and neurological factors (Mahone et al., 2018). In the field of neuropsychology, those neurodevelopmental disorders defined on the basis of behavior (including attention-deficit/hyperactivity Disorder, ADHD; learning disabilities, LDs; autism spectrum disorders, ASDs; and intellectual disability, ID) have received considerable emphasis, in part because of their prevalence and overall public health relevance (Leigh & Du, 2015; Mahone & Denckla, 2017; Mahone & Mapou, 2014). Neurodevelopmental disorders diagnosed on the basis of known or presumed medical etiologic factors have received somewhat less emphasis among neuropsychologists. Such conditions include those with genetic, environmental (injury, infection, teratogens), or multi-factorial medical etiologies.

This special issue of the Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society focuses upon such conditions with known medical or genetic etiologies, and includes 11 papers presenting innovative and novel data related to the neuropsychology (including identification of biomarkers) of specific neurodevelopmental disorders. Included in the issue are seven studies reporting new empirical findings, two critical reviews, and two case reports. The timing of this special issue follows on the heels of the 50th anniversary of the implementation of US PL-88-164 (“Mental Retardation Facilities Construction Act”), which, in 1967, provided financial support for the development 18 University Affiliated Programs (emphasizing treatment for neurodevelopmental disorders), and 12 Research Centers dedicated to research of neurodevelopmental disorders, all of which have contributed to the scientific innovations that have improved the lives of individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders and their families.

The issue begins with seven empirical studies, emphasizing disorders (both rare and more common) with genetic and associated medical etiologies, with samples ranging in age from early childhood through young adult. Williams syndrome is a rare genetic condition, often associated with intellectual disability and significant visuospatial dysfunction. In the first paper, Prieto-Corona and colleagues report on neuropsychological and functional outcomes in children with Williams syndrome, with and without the additional (even rarer) deletion of the GTF2IRD2 gene. They showed that those individuals with the additional genetic deletion had even greater dysfunction in visuospatial and social cognition, compared to those with without the deletion.

Antschel et al. report findings from a rich, 9-year longitudinal dataset of individuals with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome, a disorder associated with high risk for functional impairment and psychosis. They found that early executive function, especially working memory deficits, were associated with later functional impairment, but that the association was seen in both those with and without the disorder, highlighting the importance of early assessment of executive and cognitive control skills as predictors of later outcome.

There is considerable sexual dimorphism observed among individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. The study of individuals with sex chromosome aneuploidies—conditions characterized by abnormal numbers of X or Y chromosomes, for example, Klinefelter syndrome (XXY) or Turner syndrome (XO)—provides a highly relevant framework to investigate the etiology of some sex differences in development and function. In this issue, Udhnani and colleagues and Maiman and colleagues report on a less studied variant of sex chromosome aneuploidies—those with trisomies, tetrasomies, and pentasomies—showing an association between these variants and reductions in verbal fluency, with severity of deficits related linearly to the number of supernumerary X chromosomes.

The dystrophinopathies (including Duchenne and Becker muscular dystrophies) are X-linked muscle diseases associated with abnormal expression of the protein dystrophin. These conditions affect primarily males and result in a wide range of functional cognitive deficits. Fee and colleagues report on neuropsychological performance in a sample of 50 boys with muscular dystrophy, grouped by gene mutation position relative to exon 43. They found that boys with mutation downstream from exon 43 showed greater academic deficits, relative to those with mutation upstream of exon 43.

Medical and surgical advances contribute to an increasing number of individuals surviving congenital heart disease (CHD) and its treatment. King et al. report on neuroimaging findings in a sample of adolescents and young adults with CHD, showing reduced cerebellar volumes, with reductions predictive of executive and cognitive control functions.

The manifestation of neurobehavioral dysfunction among children with neurodevelopmental disorders often occurs early in life. Downes and colleagues present a case control study of executive functions in preschoolers with sickle cell disease (SCD). In their sample, performance-based reductions in inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility were more pronounced than parent reports of similar functions, highlighting the importance of direct assessment of executive control skills in preschoolers with SCD.

Down syndrome (DS) represents the most common genetic etiology of intellectual disability, and is associated with a wide range of medical complications and skill difficulties, especially those implicating hippocampally mediated functions. Edgin and colleagues reported minimal effects of a fast-mapping strategy, hypothesized to incrementally improve word retention, but instead showed that individuals with DS do retain novel words effectively, but only when presented during learning trials in small groups. In a related review, Hammer and colleagues provide a succinct overview of structural anatomic neuroimaging studies of individuals with DS, highlighting widespread reductions in cerebral volume early in life, with smaller effects (relative reductions) observed by adolescence.

Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a genetic neurocutaneous disorder associated with learning disabilities, ADHD, and an increased risk for brain tumors. Beaussart and colleagues provide a meta-analysis of 19 studies of individuals with NF1, emphasizing executive control skills. They concluded that, in general, working memory and planning skills were relatively more affected than inhibitory control in this population, and that relative difficulties (compared to those without NF1) tend to increase with age through adolescence.

The two final papers in this issue highlight the utility of case studies, especially in rare conditions. Tan et al. report on an individual with Pitt-Hopkins syndrome (PHS), a rare genetic disorder caused by insufficient expression of the TCF4 gene. Nearly all of the few prior published reports on PHS highlight severe intellectual and functional deficits and minimal language use. This case report instead presents findings from an individual who, despite many cognitive limitations, showed some relatively spared language function. In the final paper for this special issue, Kim et al. report on an intervention using different spacing methods to improve word list learning in a young adult with congenital amnesia secondary to premature birth and associated hypoxic-ischemic injury. They found that word recognition improved with repetitions spaced, rather than massed.

As illustrated in this set of papers, neuropsychological studies of neurodevelopmental disorders typically are conducted from a developmental perspective with an increasingly interdisciplinary approach that frequently draws upon (and informs) a refined understanding of endophenotypes and biomarkers. The ultimate hope, of course, is that these research approaches will inform more effective treatment and optimal developmental outcomes for the target populations.

It was a pleasure organizing these papers into this special issue, and we thank the authors for their contribution to this unique collection of studies demonstrating the importance of rigorous neuropsychological inquiry into neurodevelopmental conditions. It is our hope that the readers of the Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society find this collection valuable and are able to build off of the innovative and novel neuropsychological findings in the specific neurodevelopmental disorders presented within.


Individual Titles, Authors, and Articles:

Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Executive Functions in Preschool and School-Age Children With Neurofibromatosis Type 1
Author(s)
  • Marie-Laure Beaussart | Reference Center for Learning Disabilities, Nantes University Hospital, France, Laboratory of Psychology, LPPL EA4638, University of Angers, France
  • Sébastien Barbarot | Neurofibromatosis Clinic, Nantes University Hospital, France, Department of Dermatology, Nantes University Hospital, France
  • Claire Mauger | Laboratory of Psychology, LPPL EA4638, University of Angers, France
  • Arnaud Roy | Reference Center for Learning Disabilities, Nantes University Hospital, France, Laboratory of Psychology, LPPL EA4638, University of Angers, France, Neurofibromatosis Clinic, Nantes University Hospital, France

Correspondence

Disclosures
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Abstract
Objectives:

Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a genetic disorder in which the most frequent complication in children is learning disabilities.Over the past decade, growing arguments support the idea that executive dysfunction is a core deficit in children with NF1. However, some data remain inconsistent.The aim of this study was to determine the magnitude of impairment for each executive function (EF) and clarify the impact of methodological choices and participant’scharacteristics on EFs.

Methods:

In this meta-analysis, 19 studies met the selection criteria and were included with data from a total of805 children with NF1 and 667 controls. Based on the Diamond’s model (2013), EF measures were coded separately according to the following EF components: working memory,inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, planning/problem solving. The review protocol was registered with PROSPERO (International prospective register of systematicreviews; CRD42017068808).

Results:

A significant executive dysfunction in children with NF1 is demonstrated. Subgroup analysis showed thatthe impairment varied as a function of the specific component of executive functioning. The effect size for working memory and planning/problem solving was moderatewhereas it was small for inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility. Executive dysfunction seems to be greater with increasing age whereas assessment tool type,intellectual performance, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and control group composition did not seem to affect EF results.

Conclusions:

EF deficits are a core feature in children with NF1 and an early identification of executive dysfunctions is essential to limit their impact on the quality of life.(JINS, 2018, 24, 977–994)

Bibliography
  1. Asterisks mark references that are included in the meta-analysis. Google Scholar 
  2. Bernier, A., Carlson, S.M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children ’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326–339. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  3. Best, J.R., & Miller, P.H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  4. Bluschke, A., Von der Hagen, M., Papenhagen, K., Roessner, V., & Beste, C. (2017a). Conflict processing in juvenile patients with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) and healthy controls – Two pathways to success. NeuroImage: Clinical, 14, 499–505. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  5. Bluschke, A., Von der Hagen, M., Papenhagen, K., Roessner, V., & Beste, C. (2017b). Response inhibition in attention deficit disorder and neurofibromatosis type 1 – Clinically similar, neurophysiologically different. Scientific Reports, 7, 43929. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  6. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L.V., Higgins, J.P.T., & Rothstein, H.R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  7. Borenstein, M., & Higgins, J.P.T. (2013). Meta-analysis and subgroups. Prevention Science, 14(2), 134–143. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  8. Borenstein, M., Higgins, J.P.T., Hedges, L.V., & Rothstein, H.R. (2017). Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(1), 5–18. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  9. Casnar, C.L., & Klein-Tasman, B.P. (2016). Parent and teacher perspectives on emerging executive functioning in preschoolers with neurofibromatosis type 1: Comparison to unaffected children and lab-based measures. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 42(2), 198–207. Google Scholar 
  10. Castellanos, F.X., Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S., Milham, M.P., & Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3), 117–124. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  11. *Chaix, Y., Lauwers-Cancès, V., Faure-Marie, N., Gentil, C., Lelong, S., Schweitzer, E., & Castelnau, P. (2018). Deficit in phonological processes: A characteristic of the neuropsychological profile of children with NF1. Child Neuropsychology, 24(4), 558–574. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  12. Champion, J.A., Rose, K.J., Payne, J.M., Burns, J., & North, K.N. (2014). Relationship between cognitive dysfunction, gait, and motor impairment in children and adolescents with neurofibromatosis type 1. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 56(5), 468–474. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  13. Cutting, L.E., Koth, C.W., & Denckla, M.B. (2000). How children with neurofibromatosis type 1 differ from “typical” learning disabled clinic attenders: Nonverbal learning disabilities revisited. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17(1), 29–47. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  14. Cutting, L.E., Clements, A.M., Lightman, A.D., Yerby-Hammack, P.D., & Denckla, M.B. (2004). Cognitive profile of neurofibromatosis type 1: Rethinking nonverbal learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19, 155–165. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  15. Dennis, M., Francis, D.J., Cirino, P.T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M.A., & Fletcher, J.M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 15(3), 331–343. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  16. Descheemaeker, M.J., Plasschaert, E., Frijns, J.P., & Legius, E. (2013). Neuropsychological profile in adults with neurofibromatosis type 1 compared to a control group. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 57, 874–886. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  17. De Souza Costa, D., De Paula, J.J., De Rezende, N.A., Carneiro Rodrigues, L.O., Malloy-Diniz, L.F., Romano-Silva, M.A., &De Miranda, D.M. (2014). Neuropsychological impairments in elderly Neurofibromatosis type 1 patients. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 57, 216–219. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  18. Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  19. Er-Rafiqi, M., Roukoz, C., Le Gall, D., & Roy, A. (2017). Les fonctions exécutives chez l’enfant: développement, influences culturelles et perspectives cliniques. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 9(1), 27–34. Google Scholar 
  20. Ferner, R.E., Hughes, R.A.C., & Weinman, J. (1996). Intellectual impairment in neurofibromatosis 1. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 138(1–2), 125–133. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  21. *Galasso, C., Lo-Castro, A., Di Carlo, L., Pitzianti, M.B., D’Agati, E., Curatolo, P., &Pasini, A. (2014). Planning deficit in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: A neurocognitive trait independent from attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? Journal of Child Neurology, 29(10), 1320–1326. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  22. Gates, N.J., & March, E.G. (2016). A neuropsychologist’s guide to undertaking a systematic review for publication: Making the most of PRISMA guidelines. Neuropsychology Review, 26(2), 109–120. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  23. *Gilboa, Y., Josman, N., Fattal-Valevski, A., Toledano-Alhadef, H., & Rosenblum, S. (2014). Underlying mechanisms of writing difficulties among children with Neurofibromatosis type 1. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(6), 1310–1316. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  24. *Gilboa, Y., Rosenblum, S., Fattal-Valevski, A., Toledano-Alhadef, H., & Josman, N. (2014). Is there a relationship between executive functions and academic success in children with neurofibromatosis type 1? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 24(6), 918–935. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  25. Gutmann, D.H., Ferner, R.E., Listernick, R.H., Korf, B.R., Wolters, P.L., & Johnson, K.J. (2017). Neurofibromatosis type 1. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 3, 17004. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  26. Higgins, J.P.T., Thompson, S.G., Deeks, J.J., & Altman, D.G. (2003). Measuring inconsistencies in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557–560. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  27. *Huijbregts, S.C.J., Loitfelder, M., Rombouts, S.A., Swaab, H., Verbist, B.M., Arkink, E.B., & Veer, I.M. (2015). Cerebral volumetric abnormalities in Neurofibromatosis type 1: Associations with parent ratings of social and attention problems, executive dysfunction, and autistic mannerisms. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 7, 32. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  28. Hyman, S.L., Gill, D.S., Shores, E.A., Steinberg, A., Joy, P., Gibikote, S.V., &North, K.N. (2003). Natural history of cognitive deficits and their relationship to MRI T2- hyperintensities in NF1. Neurology, 60, 1139–1145. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  29. Hyman, S.L., Shores, E.A., & North, K.N. (2005). The nature and frequency of cognitive deficits in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neurology, 65, 1037–1044. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  30. Itoh, T., Magnaldi, S., White, R.M., Denckla, M.B., Hofman, K., Naidu, S., &Bryan, R.N. (1994). Neurofibromatosis type 1: The evolution of deep gray and white matter MR abnormalities. AJNR American Journal of Neuroradiology, 15, 1513–1519. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  31. Krivitzky, L.S., Walsh, K.S., Fisher, E., & Berl, M.M. (2016). Executive functioning profiles from the BRIEF across pediatric medical disorders: Age and diagnosis factors. Child Neuropsychology, 22(7), 870–888. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  32. Lacaze, E., Kieffer, V., Streri, A., Lorenzi, C., Gentaz, E., Habrand, J., & Grill, J. (2003). Neuropsychological outcome in children with optic pathway tumours when first-line treatment is chemotherapy. British Journal of Cancer, 89, 2038–2044. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  33. Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R.M.H. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development, 84, 1933–1953. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  34. *Lehtonen, A., Garg, S., Roberts, S.A., Trump, D., Evans, D.G., Green, J., &Huson, S.M. (2015). Cognition in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: Data from a population-based study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 57(7), 645–651. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  35. Lehtonen, A., Howie, E., Trump, D., & Huson, S.M. (2013). Behaviour in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: Cognition, executive function, attention, emotion, and social competence. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 55(2), 111–125. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  36. Levine, T.M., Materek, A., Abel, J., O’Donnell, M., & Cutting, L.E. (2006). Cognitive profile of neurofibromatosis type 1. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 13, 8–20. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  37. *Loitfelder, M., Huijbregts, S.C.J., Veer, I.M., Swaab, H.S., Van Buchem, M.A., Schmidt, R., &Rombouts, S.A. (2015). Functional connectivity changes and executive and social problems in neurofibromatosis type I. Brain Connectivity, 5(5), 312–320. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  38. *Lorenzo, J., Barton, B., Acosta, M.T., & North, K. (2011). Mental, motor, and language development of toddlers with neurofibromatosis type 1. Journal of Pediatrics, 158(4), 660–665. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  39. *Lorenzo, J., Barton, B., Arnold, S.S., & North, K.N. (2013). Cognitive features that distinguish preschool-age children with neurofibromatosis type 1 from their peers: A matched case-control study. Journal of Pediatrics, 163(5), 1479–1483. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  40. *Mazzocco, M.M.M., Turner, J.E., Denckla, M.B., Hofman, K.J., Scanlon, D.C., & Vellutino, F.R. (1995). Language and reading deficits associated with Neurofibromatosis Type 1: Evidence for a not-so-nonverbal learning disability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 11(4), 503–522. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  41. Moreau, N., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2014). Théorie de l’esprit et fonctions exécutives dans la pathologie. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 6(4), 276–281. Google Scholar 
  42. National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference Statement. (1988). Neurofibromatosis. Archives of Neurology, 45, 575–578. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  43. Noble, K.G., Norman, M.F., & Farah, M.J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 74–87. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  44. Pavol, M., Hiscock, M., Massman, P., Moore, B., Foorman, B., & Meyers, C. (2006). Neuropsychological function in adults with Von Recklinghausen’s neurofibromatosis. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(3), 509–526. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  45. *Payne, J.M., Arnold, S.S., Pride, N.A., & North, K.N. (2012). Does attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder exacerbate executive dysfunction in children with neurofibromatosis type 1? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54(10), 898–904. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  46. *Payne, J.M., Hyman, S.L., Shores, E.A., & North, K.N. (2011). Assessment of executive function and attention in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: Relationships between cognitive measures and real-world behavior. Child Neuropsychology, 17(4), 313–329. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  47. Payne, J.M., Pride, N.A., & North, K.N. (2016). Theory of mind in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neuropsychology, 30(4), 439–448. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  48. *Plasschaert, E., Van Eylen, L., Descheemaeker, M.J., Noens, I., Legius, E., & Steyaert, J. (2016). Executive functioning deficits in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: The influence of intellectual and social functioning. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 171, 348–362. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  49. *Pride, N., Payne, J.M., Webster, R., Shores, E.A., Rae, C., & North, K.N. (2010). Corpus callosum morphology and its relationship to cognitive function in neurofibromatosis type 1. Journal of Child Neurology, 25(7), 834–841. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  50. Pride, N.A., Payne, J.M., & North, K.N. (2012). The impact of ADHD on the cognitive and academic functioning of children with NF1. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(7), 590–600. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  51. *Remigereau, C., Roy, A., Costini, O., Barbarot, S., Bru, M., & Le Gall, D. (2017). Praxis skills and executive function in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 1–11. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  52. Ribeiro, M.J., Violante, I.R., Bernardino, I., Edden, R.A.E., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2015). Abnormal relationship between GABA, neurophysiology and impulsive behavior in neurofibromatosis type 1. Cortex, 64, 194–208. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  53. *Riva, D., Vago, C., Erbetta, A., Saletti, V., Esposito, S., Micheli, R., &Bulgheroni, S. (2017). The key search subtest of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome in Children (BADS-C) instrument reveals impaired planning without external constraints in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Journal of Child Neurology, 32(4), 387–396. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  54. Rhoades, B.L., Greenberg, M.T., Lanza, S.T., & Blair, C. (2011). Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 638–662. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  55. Rosenberg, M.S. (2005). The file-drawer problem revisited: A general weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis. Evolution, 59(2), 464–468. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  56. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  57. Rowbotham, I., Pit-Ten Cate, I.M., Sonuga-Barke, E.J., & Huijbregts, S.C. (2009). Cognitive control in adolescents with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neuropsychology, 23(1), 50–60. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  58. Roy, A. (2015). Approche neuropsychologique des fonctions exécutives de l’enfant: état des lieux et éléments de prospective. Revue de neuropsychologie, 7(4), 245–256. Google Scholar 
  59. Roy, A., Barbarot, S., Charbonnier, V., Gayet-Delacroix, M., Stalder, J.-F., Roulin, J.-L., &Le Gall, D. (2015). Examining the frontal subcortical brain vulnerability hypothesis in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: Are T2-weighted hyperintensities related to executive dysfunction? Neuropsychology, 29(3), 473–484. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  60. *Roy, A., Barbarot, S., Roulin, J.-L., Charbonnier, V., Fasotti, L., Stalder, J.-F., &Le Gall, D. (2012). Is executive function specifically impaired in children with neurofibromatosis type 1? A neuropsychological investigation of cognitive flexibility. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3, 94–102. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  61. *Roy, A., Roulin, J.-L., Charbonnier, V., Allain, P., Fasotti, L., Barbarot, S., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Executive dysfunction in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: A study of action planning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 1056–1063. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  62. Sangster, J., Shores, E.A., Watt, S., & North, K.N. (2011). The cognitive profile of preschool-aged children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Child Neuropsychology, 17(1), 1–16. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  63. Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 714–721. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  64. Shamseer, L., Moher, D., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., & Petticrew, M., . . . the PRISMA Group. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviewand meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: Elaboration and explanation. British Medical Journal, 349, g7647. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  65. Sterne, J.A.C., Sutton, A.J., Ioannidis, J.P.A., Terrin, N., Jones, D.R., Lau, J., & Higgins, J.P.T. (2011). Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 343, d4002. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  66. Stuss, D.T. (1992). Biological and psychological development of executive functions. Brain and Cognition, 20, 8–23. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  67. Toplak, M.E., West, R.F., & Stanovich, K.E. (2013). Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2), 131–143. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  68. Ullrich, N.J., Ayr, L., Leaffer, E., Irons, M.B., & Rey-Casserly, C. (2010). Pilot study of a novel computerized task to assess spatial learning in children and adolescents with neurofibromatosis type 1. Journal of Child Neurology, 25(10), 1195–1202. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  69. Van Eylen, L., Plasschaert, E., Wagemans, J., Boets, B., Legius, E., Steyaert, J., &Noens, I. (2017). Visuoperceptual processing in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: True deficit or artefact? American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 174(4), 342–358. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
Pediatric Brain Development in Down Syndrome: A Field in Its Infancy
Author(s)
  • Taralee Hamner | Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
  • Manisha D. Udhnani | Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
  • Karol Z. Osipowicz | Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
  • Nancy Raitano Lee | Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Correspondence
E-mail address | taralee.hamner@gmail.com

Disclosures
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Abstract
Objectives:

As surprisingly little is known about the developing brain studied in vivo in youth with Down syndrome(DS), the current review summarizes the small DS pediatric structural neuroimaging literature and begins to contextualize existing research within a developmentalframework.

Methods:

A systematic review of the literature was completed, effect sizes from published studies were reviewed, and results arepresented with respect to the DS cognitive behavioral phenotype and typical brain development.

Results:

The majority of DS structural neuroimagingstudies describe gross differences in brain morphometry and do not use advanced neuroimaging methods to provide nuanced descriptions of the brain. There is evidencefor smaller total brain volume (TBV), total gray matter (GM) and white matter, cortical lobar, hippocampal, and cerebellar volumes. When reductions in TBV are accountedfor, specific reductions are noted in subregions of the frontal lobe, temporal lobe, cerebellum, and hippocampus. A review of cortical lobar effect sizes reveals mostlylarge effect sizes from early childhood through adolescence. However, deviance is smaller in adolescence. Despite these smaller effects, frontal GM continues to belargely deviant in adolescence. An examination of age-frontal GM relations using effect sizes from published studies and data from Lee et al. (2016) reveals that whilethere is a strong inverse relationship between age and frontal GM volume in controls across childhood and adolescence, this is not observed in DS.

Conclusions:

Further developmentally focused research, ideally using longitudinal neuroimaging, is needed to elucidate the nature of the DS neuroanatomic phenotype during childhoodand adolescence. (JINS, 2018, 24, 966–976)

Bibliography
  1. Anderson, J.S., Nielsen, J.A., Ferguson, M.A., Burback, M.C., Cox, E.T., Dai, L., & Korenberg, J.R. (2013). Abnormal brain synchrony in Down syndrome. Neuroimage: Clinical, 2, 703–715. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.05.006 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  2. Blanken, A.E., Hurtz, S., Zarow, C., Biado, K., Honarpisheh, H., Somme, J., & Lo, D. (2017). Associations between hippocampal morphometry and neuropathologic markers of Alzheimer’s disease using 7T MRI. NeuroImage: Clinical, 15, 56–61. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  3. Buckner, R.L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron, 80(3), 807–815. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  4. Bunge, S.A., & Kahn, I. (2009). Cognition: An overview of neuroimaging techniques. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, 2, 1063–1067. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  5. Carducci, F., Onorati, P., Condoluci, C., Di Gennaro, G., Quarato, P.P., Pierallini, A., & Albertini, G. (2013). Whole-brain voxel-based morphometry study of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Functional Neurology, 28(1), 19–28. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  6. Carter, J.C., Capone, G.T., & Kaufmann, W.E. (2008). Neuroanatomic correlates of autism and stereotypy in children with Down syndrome. Neuroreport, 19(6), 653–656. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  7. Clark, C.A.C., Fernandez, F., Sakhon, S., Spano, G., & Edgin, J.O. (2017). The medial temporal memory system in Down syndrome: Translating animal models of hippocampal compromise. Hippocampus, 27(6), 683–691. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22724 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  8. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Google Scholar 
  9. Collins, D.L., Holmes, C.J., Peters, T.M., & Evans, A.C. (1995). Automatic 3‐D model‐based neuroanatomical segmentation. Human Brain Mapping, 3(3), 190–208. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  10. Fenoll, R., Pujol, J., Esteba-Castillo, S., De Sola, S., Ribas-Vidal, N., García-Alba, J., & Dierssen, M. (2017). Anomalous white matter structure and the effect of age in Down syndrome patients. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 57(1), 61–70. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  11. Fidler, D., Most, D., & Philofsky, A. (2009). The Down syndrome behavioural phenotype: Taking a developmental approach. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/253424551_The_Down_syndrome_behavioural_phenotype_Taking_a_developmental_approachGoogle Scholar 
  12. Friederici, A.D., & Gierhan, S.M. (2013). The language network. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(2), 250–254. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.10.002 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  13. Gibson, D. (1978). Down syndrome: The psychology of mongolism. London: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar 
  14. Giedd, J.N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42(4), 335–343. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.007 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  15. Giedd, J.N., Stockman, M., Weddle, C., Liverpool, M., Alexander-Bloch, A., Wallace, G.L., & Lenroot, R.K. (2010). Anatomic magnetic resonance imaging of the developing child and adolescent brain and effects of genetic variation. Neuropsychology Review, 20(4), 349–361. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9151-9 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  16. Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C., & Thompson, P.M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(21), 8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  17. Gunbey, H.P., Bilgici, M.C., Aslan, K., Has, A.C., Ogur, M.G., Alhan, A., & Lutfi, I. (2017). Structural brain alterations of Down’s syndrome in early childhood evaluation by DTI and volumetric analyses. European Journal of Radiology, 27(7), 3013–3021. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  18. Gunning‐Dixon, F.M., Brickman, A.M., Cheng, J.C., & Alexopoulos, G.S. (2009). Aging of cerebral white matter: A review of MRI findings. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(2), 109–117. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  19. Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92(1-2), 67–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.011 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  20. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163(2), 195–205. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  21. Isaacs, E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Lucas, A., Mishkin, M., & Gadian, D. (2003). Developmental amnesia and its relationship to degree of hippocampal atrophy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(22), 13060–13063. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  22. Jacola, L.M., Byars, A.W., Chalfonte-Evans, M., Schmithorst, V.J., Hickey, F., Patterson, B., & Holland, S.K. (2011). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of cognitive processing in young adults with Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116(5), 344–359. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  23. Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A.D., & Phillips, C. (2007). Long-term memory for verbal and visual information in Down syndrome and Williams syndrome: Performance on the Doors and People test. Cortex, 43(2), 233–247. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  24. Jernigan, T.L., & Bellugi, U. (1990). Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Archives of Neurology, 47(5), 529–533. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  25. Jernigan, T.L., Bellugi, U., Sowell, E., Doherty, S., & Hesselink, J.R. (1993). Cerebral morphologic distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. Archives of Neurology, 50(2), 186–191. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  26. Jung, R.E., & Haier, R.J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2), 135–154. discussion 154–187. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  27. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2012). Foreward: Development is not about studying children: The importance of longitudinal approaches. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 117(2), 87–89. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  28. Kates, W.R., Folley, B.S., Lanham, D.C., Capone, G.T., & Kaufmann, W.E. (2002). Cerebral growth in Fragile X syndrome: Review and comparison with Down syndrome. Microscopy Research and Technique, 57(3), 159–167. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  29. Kaufmann, W.E., Cooper, K.L., Mostofsky, S.H., Capone, G.T., Kates, W.R., Newschaffer, C.J., & Lanham, D.C. (2003). Specificity of cerebellar vermian abnormalities in autism: A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Child Neurology, 18(7), 463–470. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  30. Krogsrud, S.K., Fjell, A.M., Tamnes, C.K., Grydeland, H., Mork, L., Due-Tønnessen, P., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2016). Changes in white matter microstructure in the developing brain—A longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging study of children from 4 to 11years of age. NeuroImage, 124, 473–486. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  31. Lee, N.R., Adeyemi, E.I., Lin, A., Clasen, L.S., Lalonde, F.M., Condon, E., & Giedd, J.N. (2016). Dissociations in cortical morphometry in youth with Down syndrome: Evidence for reduced surface area but increased thickness. Cerebral Cortex, 26(7), 2982–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv107 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  32. Martin, G.E., Klusek, J., Estigarribia, B., & Roberts, J.E. (2009). Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. Topics in Language Disorders, 29(2), 112. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  33. McDaniel, M.A. (2005). Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. ScienceDirect, 33(4), 337–346. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005 Google Scholar 
  34. Menghini, D., Costanzo, F., & Vicari, S. (2011). Relationship between brain and cognitive processes in Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 41(3), 381–393. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9448-3 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  35. Middleton, F.A., & Strick, P.L. (2001). Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(2), 700–712. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  36. Parker, S.E., Mai, C.T., Canfield, M.A., Rickard, R., Wang, Y., Meyer, R.E., & Kirby, R.S. (2010). Updated national birth prevalence estimates for selected birth defects in the United States, 2004–2006. Birth Defects Research Part A: Clinical and Molecular Teratology, 88(12), 1008–1016. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  37. Pennington, B.F., Moon, J., Edgin, J., Stedron, J., & Nadel, L. (2003). The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: Evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Development, 74(1), 75–93. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  38. Pinter, J.D., Brown, W.E., Eliez, S., Schmitt, J.E., Capone, G.T., & Reiss, A.L. (2001). Amygdala and hippocampal volumes in children with Down syndrome: A high-resolution MRI study. Neurology, 56(7), 972–974. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  39. Pinter, J.D., Eliez, S., Schmitt, J.E., Capone, G.T., & Reiss, A.L. (2001). Neuroanatomy of Down’s syndrome: A high-resolution MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(10), 1659–1665. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  40. Powell, D., Caban-Holt, A., Jicha, G., Robertson, W., Davis, R., Gold, B.T., & Head, E. (2014). Frontal white matter integrity in adults with Down syndrome with and without dementia. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(7), 1562–1569. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.01.137 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  41. Ramnani, N. (2006). The primate cortico-cerebellar system: Anatomy and function. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 7(7), 511–522. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  42. Raschle, N., Zuk, J., Ortiz-Mantilla, S., Sliva, D.D., Franceschi, A., Grant, P.E., & Gaab, N. (2012). Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: Challenges and practical guidelines. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 43–50. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  43. Savage, L.M., Buzzetti, R.A., & Ramirez, D.R. (2004). The effects of hippocampal lesions on learning, memory, and reward expectancies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82(2), 109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.05.002 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  44. Shaw, P., Gogtay, N., & Rapoport, J. (2010). Childhood psychiatric disorders as anomalies in neurodevelopmental trajectories. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6), 917–925. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21028 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  45. Smigielska-Kuzia, J., Bockowski, L., Sobaniec, W., Sendrowski, K., Olchowik, B., Cholewa, M., & Lebkowska, U. (2011). A volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study of brain structures in children with Down syndrome. Polish Journal of Neurology and Neurosurgery, 45(4), 363–369. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  46. Sowell, E.R., Peterson, B.S., Thompson, P.M., Welcome, S.E., Henkenius, A.L., & Toga, A.W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 309–315. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  47. Spanò, M., Mercuri, E., Randò, T., Pantò, T., Gagliano, A., Henderson, S., & Guzetta, F. (1999). Motor and perceptual–motor competence in children with Down syndrome: Variation in performance with age. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 3(1), 7–14. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  48. Squire, L.R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychology Review, 99(2), 195–231. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  49. Stiles, J., & Jernigan, T.L. (2010). The basics of brain development. Neuropsychology Review, 20(4), 327–348. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  50. Thomas, M.S., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Scerif, G., Jarrold, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 336–358. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  51. Thompson, P.M., Stein, J.L., Medland, S.E., Hibar, D.P., Vasquez, A.A., Renteria, M.E., & Franke, B. (2014). The ENIGMA Consortium: Large-scale collaborative analyses of neuroimaging and genetic data. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(2), 153–182. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  52. Tiemeier, H., Lenroot, R.K., Greenstein, D.K., Tran, L., Pierson, R., & Giedd, J.N. (2010). Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal morphometric MRI study. NeuroImage, 49(1), 63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.016 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  53. Uematsu, A., Matsui, M., Tanaka, C., Takahashi, T., Noguchi, K., Suzuki, M., & Nishijo, H. (2012). Developmental trajectories of amygdala and hippocampus from infancy to early adulthood in healthy individuals. PLoS One, 7(10), e46970. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  54. van den Heuvel, M.P., & Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 683–696. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  55. Vicari, S. (2006). Motor development and neuropsychological patterns in persons with Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 36(3), 355–364. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  56. Volman, M.J., Visser, J.J., & Lensvelt-Mulders, G.J. (2007). Functional status in 5 to 7-year-old children with Down syndrome in relation to motor ability and performance mental ability. Disability and Rehabilitation, 29(1), 25–31. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  57. Webb, S.J., Monk, C.S., & Nelson, C.A. (2001). Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: Implications for human development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(2), 147–171. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1902_2 CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  58. Weiner, M.W., Veitch, D.P., Aisen, P.S., Beckett, L.A., Cairns, N.J., Green, R.C., & Liu, E. (2013). The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: A review of papers published since its inception. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 9(5), e111–e194. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  59. Zigman, W.B., & Lott, I.T. (2007). Alzheimer’s disease in Down syndrome: Neurobiology and risk. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(3), 237–246. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
Executive Skills and Academic Achievement in the Dystrophinopathies
Author(s)
  • Robert J. Fee | The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Queens College, Queens, New York
  • Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Columbia University, New York, New York
  • Jacqueline Montes | Departments of Neurology and Rehabilitation and Regenerative Medicine, Columbia University, New York, New York
  • Jennifer L. Stewart | The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Queens College, Queens, New York
  • Veronica J. Hinton | The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Queens College, Queens, New York
  • Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center and Department of Neurology, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York

Correspondence
E-mail address | vjh9@columbia.edu

Disclosures
Fee, R.J., Montes, J, Stewart, J.L., and Hinton, V.J. declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Abstract

Objectives:

To examine academic performance in dystrophinopathy as a function of dystrophin gene mutation position as well as intellectual function,executive skills, socioeconomic status (SES), behavior, and physical ability.

Methods:

In a cross-sectional study, boys with dystrophinopathy(ages 5–17; n=50) completed tests of academics (Woodcock-Johnson-III: spelling, reading, calculation and total scores), executive functioning(selective attention/inhibitory control, set shifting, working memory, and processing speed), single word comprehension and nonverbal reasoning. Motor skills wereassessed and parents provided demographic information and child behavioral assessments. Dystrophin gene mutation positions were dichotomized into groups (upstreamversus downstream of exon 43, location of isoforms previously linked to intellectual impairment). Genetic mutation groups were compared on measures of academic achievement,and multiple regression analyses examined unique and joint contributions of executive skills, intelligence quotient (IQ), SES, motor abilities, behavior, and mutationpositions to academic outcomes.

Results:

Academic performance was slightly, yet significantly, lower than IQ and varied as a function ofdystrophin gene position, wherein boys possessing the downstream mutation exhibited greater impairment than boys with the upstream mutation. Digit span forward (indexingverbal span), but no other measure of executive function, contributed significant variance to total academic achievement, spelling and calculation.

Conclusions:

Weak academic performance is associated with dystrophinopathy and is more common in downstream mutations. A specific deficit in verbal span may underlie inefficienciesobserved in children with dystrophinopathy and may drive deficits impacting academic abilities. (JINS, 2018, 24,928–938)

Bibliography
  1. Allen, D.G., Whitehead, N.P., & Froehner, S.C. (2016). Absence of dystrophin disrupts skeletal muscle signaling: Roles of Ca2+, Reactive oxygen species, and nitric oxide in the development of muscular dystrophy. Physiological Reviews, 96(1), 253–305. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  2. Alloway, T.P., & Alloway, R.G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 20–29. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  3. Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., & Morley, J.W. (2003). Altered inhibitory input to Purkinje cells of dystrophin-deficient mice. Brain Research, 982(2), 280–283. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  4. Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., & Morley, J.W. (2004). Long-term depression is reduced in cerebellar Purkinje cells of dystrophin-deficient mdx mice. Brain Research, 1019(1-2), 289–292. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  5. Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., & Morley, J.W. (2012). Duchenne muscular dystrophy and brain function. In D.M. Hegde (Ed.), Muscular dystrophy. London: InTech. Google Scholar 
  6. Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., Rae, C., & Morley, J.W. (2002). Brain function in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Brain, 125(Pt 1), 4–13. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  7. Astrea, G., Pecini, C., Gasperini, F., Brisca, G., Scutifero, M., Bruno, C., & Battini, R. (2015). Reading impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A pilot study to investigate similarities and differences with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 45-46, 168–177. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  8. Bardoni, A., Felisari, G., Sironi, M., Comi, G., Lai, M., Robotti, M., & Bresolin, N. (2000). Loss of Dp140 regulatory sequences is associated with cognitive impairment in dystrophinopathies. Neuromuscular Disorders, 10(3), 194–199. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  9. Best, J.R., Miller, P.H., & Naglieri, J.A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  10. Billard, C., Gillet, P., Barthez, M., Hommet, C., & Bertrand, P. (1998). Reading ability and processing in Duchenne muscular dystrophy and spinal muscular atrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40(1), 12–20. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  11. Billard, C., Gillet, P., Signoret, J.L., Uicaut, E., Bertrand, P., Fardeau, M., & Santini, J.J. (1992). Cognitive functions in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A reappraisal and comparison with spinal muscular atrophy. Neuromuscular Disorders, 2(5-6), 371–378. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  12. Bradley, R.H., & Corwyn, R.F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371–399. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  13. Brooke, M.H., Griggs, R.C., Mendell, J.R., Fenichel, G.M., Shumate, J.B., & Pellegrino, R.J. (1981). Clinical trial in Duchenne dystrophy. I. The design of the protocol. Muscle Nerve, 4(3), 186–197. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  14. Clark, C.A., Pritchard, V.E., & Woodward, L.J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  15. Cotton, S., Voudouris, N.J., & Greenwood, K.M. (2001). Intelligence and Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Full-scale, verbal, and performance intelligence quotients. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43(7), 497–501. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  16. Cutting, L.E., Materek, A., Cole, C.A., Levine, T.M., & Mahone, E.M. (2009). Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 34–54. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  17. Cyrulnik, S.E., Fee, R.J., Batchelder, A., Kiefel, J., Goldstein, E., & Hinton, V.J. (2008). Cognitive and adaptive deficits in young children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD). Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(5), 853–861. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  18. Daoud, F., Angeard, N., Demerre, B., Martie, I., Benyaou, R., Leturcq, F., & Tuffery, S. (2009). Analysis of Dp71 contribution in the severity of mental retardation through comparison of Duchenne and Becker patients differing by mutation consequences on Dp71 expression. Human Molecular Genetics, 18(20), 3779–3794. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  19. DeStefano, D., & LeFevre, J.A. (2004). The role of working memory in mental arithmetic. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(3), 353–386. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  20. Donders, J., & Taneja, C. (2009). Neurobehavioral characteristics of children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 295–304. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  21. Dorman, C., Hurley, A.D., & D’Avignon, J. (1988). Language and learning disorders of older boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 30(3), 316–327. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  22. Duncan, G.J., & Murnane, R.J. (2011). Whither opportunity?: Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Google Scholar 
  23. Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-IV) (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. Google Scholar 
  24. Fabbro, F., Marini, A., Felisari, G., Comi, G.P., D’Angelo, M.G., Turconi, A.C., & Bresolin, N. (2007). Language disturbances in a group of participants suffering from Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A pilot study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104(2), 663–676. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  25. Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–16. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  26. Geary, D.C., Hoard, M.K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development, 78(4), 1343–1359. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  27. Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Google Scholar 
  28. Gorecki, D.C., Lukasiuk, K., Szklarczyk, A., & Kaczmarek, L. (1998). Kainate-evoked changes in dystrophin messenger RNA levels in the rat hippocampus. Neuroscience, 84(2), 467–477. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  29. Hendriksen, J.G., & Vles, J.S. (2006). Are males with Duchenne muscular dystrophy at risk for reading disabilities? Pediatric Neurology, 34(4), 296–300. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  30. Hinton, V.J., De Vivo, D.C., Fee, R.J., Goldstein, E., & Stern, Y. (2004). Investigation of poor academic achievement in children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19(3), 146–154. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  31. Hinton, V.J., De Vivo, D.C., Nereo, N.E., Goldstein, E., & Stern, Y. (2000). Poor verbal working memory across intellectual level in boys with Duchenne dystrophy. Neurology, 54(11), 2127–2132. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  32. Hinton, V.J., De Vivo, D.C., Nereo, N.E., Goldstein, E., & Stern, Y. (2001). Selective deficits in verbal working memory associated with a known genetic etiology: The neuropsychological profile of duchenne muscular dystrophy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(1), 45–54. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  33. Hinton, V.J., Fee, R., Goldstein, E.M., & De Vivo, D.C. (2007). Verbal and memory skills in males with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(2), 123–128. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  34. Hodes, R.J., Insel, T.R., Landis, S.C. & NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research. (2013). The NIH toolbox: Setting a standard for biomedical research. Neurology, 80(11 Suppl 3), S1. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  35. Kim, T.W., Wu, K., & Black, I.B. (1995). Deficiency of brain synaptic dystrophin in human Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Annals of Neurology, 38(3), 446–449. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  36. Komarraju, M., & Nadler, D. (2013). Self-efficacy and academic achievement: Why do implicit beliefs, goals, and effort regulation matter? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 67–72. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  37. Leaffer, E.B., Fee, R.J., & Hinton, V.J. (2016). Digit span performance in children with dystrophinopathy: A verbal span or working memory contribution? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(7), 777–784. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  38. Leibowitz, D., & Dubowitz, V. (1981). Intellect and behaviour in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 23(5), 577–590. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  39. Lenk, U., Hanke, R., Thiele, H., & Speer, A. (1993). Point mutations at the carboxy terminus of the human dystrophin gene: implications for an association with mental retardation in DMD patients. Human Molecular Genetics, 2(11), 1877–1881. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  40. Lidov, H.G. (1996). Dystrophin in the nervous system. Brain Pathology, 6(1), 63–77. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  41. Mayes, S.D., Calhoun, S.L., Bixler, E.O., & Zimmerman, D.N. (2009). IQ and neuropsychological predictors of academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 238–241. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  42. Mazzocco, M.M., & Kover, S.T. (2007). A longitudinal assessment of executive function skills and their association with math performance. Child Neuropsychology, 13(1), 18–45. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  43. McGrew, K.S., Flanagan, D.P., Keith, T.Z., & Vanderwood, M. (1997). Beyond g: The impact of Gf-Gc specific cognitive abilities research on the future use and interpretation of intelligence tests in the schools. School Psychology Review, 26, 189–210. Google Scholar 
  44. Mehler, M.F. (2000). Brain dystrophin, neurogenetics and mental retardation. Brain Research Reviews, 32(1), 277–307. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  45. Mento, G., Tarantino, V., & Bisiacchi, P.S. (2011). The neuropsychological profile of infantile Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(8), 1359–1377. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  46. Moizard, M.P., Toutain, A., Fournier, D., Berret, F., Raynaud, M., Billard, C., & Moraine, C. (2000). Severe cognitive impairment in DMD: Obvious clinical indication for Dp71 isoform point mutation screening. European Journal of Human Genetics, 8(7), 552–556. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  47. Pinquart, M., & Teubert, D. (2011). Academic, physical, and social functioning of children and adolescents with chronic physical illness: A meta-analysis. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 37(4), 376–389. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  48. Rasic, M.V., Vojinovic, D., Pesovic, J., Mijalkovic, G., Lukic, V., Mladenovic, J., & Romac, S. (2014). Intellectual ability in the Duchenne muscular dystrophy and dystrophin gene mutation location. Balkan Journal of Medical Genetics, 17(2), 25–35. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  49. Reynolds, C.R., & Kamphaus, R.W. (2004). BASC-2: Behavior assessment system for children. Toronto: Pearson Canada Assessment. Google Scholar 
  50. Ricotti, V., Mandy, W.P., Scoto, M., Pane, M., Deconinck, N., Messina, S., & Muntoni, F. (2016). Neurodevelopmental, emotional, and behavioural problems in Duchenne muscular dystrophy in relation to underlying dystrophin gene mutations. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 58(1), 77–84. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  51. Rossen, E.A., Shearer, D.K., Penfield, R.D., & Kranzler, J.H. (2005). Validity of the comprehensive test of nonverbal intelligence (CTONI). Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23(2), 161–172. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  52. Sesma, H.W., Mahone, E.M., Levine, T., Eason, S.H., & Cutting, L.E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15(3), 232–246. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  53. Sogos, V., Curto, M., Reali, C., & Gremo, F. (2002). Developmentally regulated expression and localization of dystrophin and utrophin in the human fetal brain. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development, 123(5), 455–462. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  54. St Clair-Thompson, H.L., & Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745–759. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  55. Swanson, H.L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(3), 294–321. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  56. Taras, H., & Potts‐Datema, W. (2005). Chronic health conditions and student performance at school. Journal of School Health 75(7), 255–266. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  57. Taylor, P.J., Betts, G.A., Maroulis, S., Gilissen, C., Pedersen, R.L., Mowat, D.R., & Buckley, M.F. (2010). Dystrophin gene mutation location and the risk of cognitive impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Public Library of Science One, 5(1), e8803. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  58. Tuffery, S., Lenk, U., Roberts, R.G., Coubes, C., Demaille, J., & Claustres, M. (1995). Protein truncation test: Analysis of two novel point mutations at the carboxy terminus of the human dystrophin gene associated with mental retardation. Human Mutation, 6(2), 126–135. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  59. van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P.F., & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35(5), 427–449. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  60. Vignos, P.J., Spencer, G.E., & Archibald, K.C. (1963). Management of progressive muscular dystrophy of childhood. Journal of the American Medical Association, 184(2), 89–96. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  61. Waite, A., Brown, S.C., & Blake, D.J. (2012). The dystrophin–glycoprotein complex in brain development and disease. Trends in Neurosciences, 35(8), 487–496. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  62. Wang, M.-T., & Eccles, J.S. (2013). School context, achievement motivation, and academic engagement: A longitudinal study of school engagement using a multidimensional perspective. Learning and Instruction, 28, 12–23. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  63. Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV(WISC-IV) Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. Google Scholar 
  64. Weintraub, S., Bauer, P.J., Zelazo, P.D., Wallner-Allen, K., Dikmen, S.S., Heaton, R.K., & Gershon, R.C. (2013). NIH toolbox cognition battery: Introduction and pediatric data. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 1–15. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  65. Welsh, J.A., Nix, R.L., Blair, C., Bierman, K.L., & Nelson, K.E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  66. Wentzel, K.R. (1993). Does being good make the grade? Social behavior and academic competence in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 357. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  67. Wicksell, R.K., Kihlgren, M., Melin, L., & Eeg-Olofsson, O. (2004). Specific cognitive deficits are common in children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46(3), 154–159. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  68. Wingeier, K., Giger, E., Strozzi, S., Kreis, R., Joncourt, F., Conrad, B., & Steinlin, M. (2011). Neuropsychological impairments and the impact of dystrophin mutations on general cognitive functioning of patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 18(1), 90–95. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  69. Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III: Tests of Academic and Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. Google Scholar 
  70. Worden, D.K., & Vignos, P.J. Jr., (1962). Intellectual function in childhood progressive muscular dystrophy. Pediatrics, 29, 968–977. Google Scholar  PubMed 
Small Sets of Novel Words Are Fully Retained After 1-Week in Typically Developing Children and Down Syndrome: A Fast Mapping Study
Author(s)
  • Stella Sakhon | Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona
  • Kelly Edwards | Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona
  • Alison Luongo | Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona
  • Melanie Murphy | Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona
  • Jamie Edgin | Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona

Correspondence
E-mail address | jedgin@email.arizona.edu

Disclosures
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

Abstract
Objectives:

Down syndrome (DS) is a population with known hippocampal impairment, with studies showing that individuals with DS display difficultiesin spatial navigation and remembering arbitrary bindings. Recent research has also demonstrated the importance of the hippocampus for novel word-learning. Based onthese data, we aimed to determine whether individuals with DS show deficits in learning new labels and if they may benefit from encoding conditions thought to be lessreliant on hippocampal function (i.e., through fast mapping).

Methods:

In the current study, we examined immediate, 5-min, and 1-week delayedword-learning across two learning conditions (e.g., explicit encoding vs. fast mapping). These conditions were examined across groups (twenty-six3- to 5-year-old typically developing children and twenty-six 11- to 28-year-old individuals with DS with comparable verbal and nonverbal scores on the Kaufman BriefIntelligence Test – second edition) and in reference to sleep quality.

Results:

Both individuals with and without DS showed retention aftera 1-week delay, and the current study found no benefit of the fast mapping condition in either group contrary to our expectations. Eye tracking data showed that preferentialeye movements to target words were not present immediately but emerged after 1-week in both groups. Furthermore, sleep measures collected via actigraphy did not relate to retention in either group.

Conclusions:

This study presents novel data on long-term knowledge retention in referenceto sleep patterns in DS and adds to a body of knowledge helping us to understand the processes of word-learning in typical and atypically developing populations. (JINS, 2018, 24, 955–965)

Bibliography
  1. Ábrahám, H., Vincze, A., Veszprémi, B., Kravják, A., Gömöri, É., Kovács, G.G., & Seress, L. (2012). Impaired myelination of the human hippocampal formation in Down syndrome. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(2), 147–158. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  2. Ashworth, A., Hill, C.M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Dimitriou, D. (2015). A cross-syndrome study of the differential effects of sleep on declarative memory consolidation in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Developmental Science, 20(2), 1–13. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  3. Atir-Sharon, T., Gilboa, A., Hazan, H., Koilis, E., & Manevitz, L.M. (2015). Decoding the formation of new semantics: MVPA investigation of rapid neocortical plasticity during associative encoding through Fast Mapping. Neural Plasticity, 2015, 1–15. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  4. Bird, E.K.R., Chapman, R.S., & Schwartz, S.E. (2004). Fast mapping of words and story recall by individuals with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1286–1300. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  5. Brady, K.W., & Goodman, J.C. (2014). The type, but not the amount, of information available influences toddlers’ fast mapping and retention of new words. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), 120–133. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  6. Breslin, J., Spanò, G., Bootzin, R., Anand, P., Nadel, L., & Edgin, J. (2014). Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome and cognition in Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 56(7), 657–664. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  7. Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17–29. Google Scholar 
  8. Chapman, R.S., Bird, E.K.R., & Schwartz, S.E. (1990). Fast mapping of words in event contexts by children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(4), 761–770. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  9. Churchill, S.S., Kieckhefer, G.M., Landis, C.A., & Ward, T.M. (2012). Sleep measurement and monitoring in children with Down syndrome: A review of the literature, 1960–2010. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16(5), 477–488. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  10. Clark, C.A., Fernandez, F., Sakhon, S., Spanò, G., & Edgin, J.O. (2017). The medial temporal memory system in Down syndrome: Translating animal models of hippocampal compromise. Hippocampus, 27(6), 683–691. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  11. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  12. Courbois, Y., Farran, E.K., Lemahieu, A., Blades, M., Mengue-Topio, H., & Sockeel, P. (2013). Wayfinding behaviour in Down syndrome: A study with virtual environments. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(5), 1825–1831. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  13. Coutanche, M.N., & Thompson-Schill, S.L. (2014). Fast mapping rapidly integrates information into existing memory networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(6), 2296. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  14. Davachi, L., & Wagner, A.D. (2002). Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: Insights from relational and item-based learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88(2), 982–990. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  15. Davis, M.H., & Gaskell, M.G. (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3773–3800. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  16. Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 114–126. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  17. Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N.J. (2006). Development of shared information in communication despite hippocampal amnesia. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 140. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  18. Eichenbaum, H., & Bunsey, M. (1995). On the binding of associations in memory: Clues from studies on the role of the hippocampal region in paired-associate learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(1), 19–23. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  19. Edgin, J.O. (2013). Cognition in Down syndrome: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(3), 307–317. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  20. Edgin, J.O., Tooley, U., Demara, B., Nyhuis, C., Anand, P., & Spanò, G. (2015). Sleep disturbance and expressive language development in preschool‐age children with Down syndrome. Child Development, 86(6), 1984–1998. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  21. Feld, G.B., Weis, P.P., & Born, J. (2016). The limited capacity of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1368. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  22. Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., & Reznick, S.J. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego: Singular Publishing. Google Scholar 
  23. Giovanello, K.S., Schnyer, D., & Verfaellie, M. (2009). Distinct hippocampal regions make unique contributions to relational memory. Hippocampus, 19(2), 111–117. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  24. Gómez, R.L., & Edgin, J.O. (2015). Sleep as a window into early neural development: Shifts in sleep‐dependent learning effects across early childhood. Child Development Perspectives, 9(3), 183–189. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  25. Gómez, R.L., & Edgin, J.O. (2016). The extended trajectory of hippocampal development: Implications for early memory development and disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 57–69. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  26. Greve, A., Cooper, E., & Henson, R.N. (2014). No evidence that ‘fast-mapping’ benefits novel learning in healthy older adults. Neuropsychologia, 60, 52–59. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  27. Hannula, D.E., & Ranganath, C. (2009). The eyes have it: Hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in the eye movements. Neuron, 63(5), 592–599. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  28. Horst, J.S., & Samuelson, L.K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-month-old infants. Infancy, 13(2), 128–157. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  29. Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A.D., & Phillips, C. (2007). Long-term memory for verbal and visual information in Down syndrome and Williams syndrome: Performance on the Doors and People test. Cortex, 43(2), 233–247. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  30. Lavenex, P.B., Bostelmann, M., Brandner, C., Costanzo, F., Fragnière, E., Klencklen, G., & Vicari, S. (2015). Allocentric spatial learning and memory deficits in Down syndrome. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–17. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  31. Lejeune, J., Gautier, M., & Turpin, R. (1959). Study of somatic chromosomes from 9 mongoloid children. Comptes rendus hebdomadaires des seances de l’Academie des sciences, 248(11), 1721. Google Scholar  PubMed 
  32. Markson, L., & Bloom, P. (1997). Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature, 385(6619), 813–815. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  33. Mayes, A.R., Holdstock, J.S., Isaac, C.L., Montaldi, D., Grigor, J., Gummer, A., & Gong, Q. (2004). Associative recognition in a patient with selective hippocampal lesions and relatively normal item recognition. Hippocampus, 14(6), 763–784. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  34. McClelland, J.L. (2013). Incorporating rapid neocortical learning of new schema-consistent information into complementary learning systems theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1190. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  35. McClelland, J.L., McNaughton, B.L., & O’Reilly, R.C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102(3), 419. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  36. Meltzer, L.J., Montgomery-Downs, H.E., Insana, S.P., & Walsh, C.M. (2012). Use of actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16(5), 463–475. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  37. Menghini, D., Costanzo, F., & Vicari, S. (2011). Relationship between brain and cognitive processes in Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 41(3), 381–393. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  38. Merhav, M., Karni, A., & Gilboa, A. (2015). Not all declarative memories are created equal: Fast mapping as a direct route to cortical declarative representations. Neuroimage, 117, 80–92. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  39. Mervis, C.B., & Robinson, B.F. (2000). Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: A comparison. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17(1), 111–126. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  40. Milojevich, H., & Lukowski, A. (2016). Recall memory in children with Down syndrome and typically developing peers matched on developmental age. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 60(1), 89–100. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  41. Nadel, L. (2003). Down’s syndrome: A genetic disorder in biobehavioral perspective. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 2(3), 156–166. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  42. Pennington, B.F., Moon, J., Edgin, J., Stedron, J., & Nadel, L. (2003). The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: Evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Development, 74(1), 75–93. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  43. Richmond, J., & Nelson, C.A. (2009). Relational memory during infancy: Evidence from eye tracking. Developmental Science, 12(4), 549–556. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  44. Roberts, L.V., & Richmond, J.L. (2015). Preschoolers with Down syndrome do not yet show the learning and memory impairments seen in adults with Down syndrome. Developmental science, 18(3), 404–419. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  45. Roediger, H.L. III, & Karpicke, J.D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3), 181–210. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  46. Sadeh, A. (2011). The role and validity of actigraphy in sleep medicine: An update. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 15(4), 259–267. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  47. Sharon, T., Moscovitch, M., & Gilboa, A. (2011). Rapid neocortical acquisition of long-term arbitrary associations independent of the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(3), 1146–1151. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  48. Singer Harris, N.G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 345–370. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  49. Smith, G.K., Kesner, R.P., & Korenberg, J.R. (2014). Dentate gyrus mediates cognitive function in the Ts65Dn/DnJ mouse model of Down syndrome. Hippocampus, 24(3), 354–362. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  50. Smith, C.N., Urgolites, Z.J., Hopkins, R.O., & Squire, L.R. (2014). Comparison of explicit and incidental learning strategies in memory-impaired patients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(1), 475–479. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  51. Spiegel, C., & Halberda, J. (2011). Rapid fast-mapping abilities in 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(1), 132–140. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  52. Vlach, H.A., & Sandhofer, C.M. (2012). Fast mapping across time: Memory processes support children’s retention of learned words. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 46. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  53. Wang, J.Y., Weber, F.D., Zinke, K., Inostroza, M., & Born, J. (2017). More effective consolidation of episodic long‐term memory in children than adults—Unrelated to sleep. Child Development. doi:10.1111/cdev.12839 CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  54. Warren, D.E., & Duff, M.C. (2014). Not so fast: Hippocampal amnesia slows word learning despite successful fast mapping. Hippocampus, 24(8), 920–933. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  55. Warren, D.E., Tranel, D., & Duff, M.C. (2016). Impaired acquisition of new words after left temporal lobectomy despite normal fast-mapping behavior. Neuropsychologia, 80, 165–175. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  56. Waxman, S.R., & Booth, A.E. (2000). Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts. Cognition, 77(2), B33–B43. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  57. Wisniewski, K.E. (1990). Down syndrome children often have brain with maturation delay, retardation of growth, and cortical dysgenesis. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 37(S7), 274–281. doi:10.1002/ajmg.1320370755 CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  58. Yoder, P., Woynaroski, T., Fey, M., & Warren, S. (2014). Effects of dose frequency of early communication intervention in young children with and without Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 119(1), 17–32. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed 
  59. Yonelinas, A.P., Hopfinger, J.B., Buonocore, M.H., Kroll, N.E.A., & Baynes, K. (2001). Hippocampal, parahippocampal and occipital-temporal contributions to associative and item recognition memory: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(2), 359–363. CrossRef  Google Scholar 
  60. Yonelinas, A.P. (2013). The hippocampus supports high-resolution binding in the service of perception, working memory and long-term memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 254, 34–44. CrossRef  Google Scholar  PubMed